DMCA
Review Review Review Review Connectionist psycholinguistics: capturing the empirical data
Citations
3694 | Learning internal representations by error propagation - Rumelhart, Hinton, et al. - 1986 |
2068 | Finding structure in time
- Elman
- 1990
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ple, capture aspects of language and language processing. These models showed that connectionist networks could acquire parts of linguistic structure without extensive ‘innate’ knowledge1. Recent work has moved towards a ‘connectionist psycholinguistics’, which captures detailed psychological data2. Criteria for connectionist psycholinguistics We review progress in connectionist psycholinguistics in four key areas: speech processing, sentence processing, language production, and reading aloud. We suggest that computational models, whether connectionist or symbolic, should meet three criteria: (1) data contact, (2) task veridicality, and (3) input representativeness. Data contact refers to the degree to which a model captures psycholinguistic data. Of course, there is more to capturing the data than simply fitting existing empirical results; for example, a model should also make non-obvious predictions (see Ref. 3 for discussion). Task veridicality refers to the match between the task facing people and the task given to the model. Although a precise match is difficult to obtain, it is important to minimize the discrepancy. For example, many models of the English past tense4 have low ta... |
706 | A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Brain and Language - Seidenberg, McClelland - 1989 |
698 | A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory - Just, Carpenter - 1992 |
611 | Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains - Plaut, McClelland, et al. - 1996 |
591 | The TRACE Model of Speech Perception - McClelland, Elman - 1986 |
573 |
On Learning the Past Tenses of English Verbs
- Rumelhart, McClelland
- 1986
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...heard before. Generalization to new cases is thus a crucial testa for many connectionist models. (3) Representation Because most connectionist nets learn, their internal codes are devised by the network to be appropriate for the task. Developing methods for understanding these codes is an important research strand. Whereas internal codes may be learned, the inputs and outputs to a network generally use a code specified by the researcher. The choice of code can be crucial in determining network performance. How these codes relate to standard symbolic representations of language is contentious. (4) Rules versus exceptions Many aspects of language exhibit ‘quasi-regularities’: regularities which usually hold, but which admit exceptions. In a symbolic framework, quasi-regularities may be captured by symbolic rules, associated with explicit lists of exceptions. Symbolic processing models often incorporate this distinction by having separate mechanisms for regular and exceptional cases. In contrast, connectionist nets may provide a single mechanism that can learn general rule-like regularities, and their exceptions. The viability of such ‘single route’ models has been a major point of contr... |
501 | Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies - Gibson - 1998 |
414 | On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition - Pinker, Prince - 1988 |
402 | Distributed Representations, Simple Recurrent Networks - Elman - 1991 |
322 | Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition - Norris - 1994 |
271 | 3). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – A causal connection - Bradley, Bryant - 1983 |
269 | Rules of language. - Pinker - 1991 |
214 |
How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing.
- Roberts, Pashler
- 2000
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... processing. These models showed that connectionist networks could acquire parts of linguistic structure without extensive ‘innate’ knowledge1. Recent work has moved towards a ‘connectionist psycholinguistics’, which captures detailed psychological data2. Criteria for connectionist psycholinguistics We review progress in connectionist psycholinguistics in four key areas: speech processing, sentence processing, language production, and reading aloud. We suggest that computational models, whether connectionist or symbolic, should meet three criteria: (1) data contact, (2) task veridicality, and (3) input representativeness. Data contact refers to the degree to which a model captures psycholinguistic data. Of course, there is more to capturing the data than simply fitting existing empirical results; for example, a model should also make non-obvious predictions (see Ref. 3 for discussion). Task veridicality refers to the match between the task facing people and the task given to the model. Although a precise match is difficult to obtain, it is important to minimize the discrepancy. For example, many models of the English past tense4 have low task veridicality because they map verb stems t... |
190 | Learning state space trajectories in recurrent neural networks. - Pearlmutter - 1989 |
179 | Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension - McRae, Spivey-Knowlton, et al. - 1998 |
170 | Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers - Dell, Schwartz, et al. - 1997 |
169 | Toward a connectionist model of recursion in human linguistic performance. - Christiansen, Chater - 1999 |
164 | Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. - Harm, Seidenberg - 1999 |
154 | Learning to segment speech using multiple cues: A connectionist model - Christiansen, Allen, et al. - 1998 |
145 | An efficient gradient-based algorithm for online training of recurrent network trajectories,” - Williams, Peng - 1990 |
117 | A computational learning model for metrical phonology. - Dresher, Kaye - 1990 |
94 | Is compensation for coarticulation mediated by the lexicon? - Pitt, McQueen - 1998 |
92 | A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. - Seidenberg, MacDonald - 1999 |
90 | Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes - Elman, McClelland - 1988 |
83 | Parsing in a dynamical system: an attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints in sentence processing. - Tabor - 1997 |
82 | Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics - Linebarger, Schwartz, et al. - 1983 |
80 | Rethinking eliminative connectionism. - Marcus - 1998 |
75 | Subsymbolic case-role analysis of sentences with embedded clauses. - Miikkulainen - 1996 |
68 | On the connection between connectionism and data: are a few words necessary? - Besner - 1990 |
62 | Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. - Zorzi, Houghton, et al. - 1998 |
56 | Constraints on sentence comprehension - Gibson, Pearlmutter - 1998 |
55 | Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. - Weekes - 1997 |
54 | Crossed and nested dependencies in German and Dutch: a psycholinguistic study. - Bach - 1986 |
53 | Influence of preceding fricative on stop consonant perception - Mann, Repp - 1981 |
47 | Dynamical models of sentence processing - Tabor, Tanenhaus - 1999 |
47 | Connectionist models of language production: lexical access and grammatical encoding. - Dell - 1999 |
44 | Connectionist natural language processing: the state of the art.
- Christiansen, Chater
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ts of language and language processing. These models showed that connectionist networks could acquire parts of linguistic structure without extensive ‘innate’ knowledge1. Recent work has moved towards a ‘connectionist psycholinguistics’, which captures detailed psychological data2. Criteria for connectionist psycholinguistics We review progress in connectionist psycholinguistics in four key areas: speech processing, sentence processing, language production, and reading aloud. We suggest that computational models, whether connectionist or symbolic, should meet three criteria: (1) data contact, (2) task veridicality, and (3) input representativeness. Data contact refers to the degree to which a model captures psycholinguistic data. Of course, there is more to capturing the data than simply fitting existing empirical results; for example, a model should also make non-obvious predictions (see Ref. 3 for discussion). Task veridicality refers to the match between the task facing people and the task given to the model. Although a precise match is difficult to obtain, it is important to minimize the discrepancy. For example, many models of the English past tense4 have low task veridicality be... |
35 | Does lexical information influence the perceptual restoration of phonemes? - Samuel - 1996 |
34 | Bottom-up connectionist models of ‘interaction’. - Norris - 1993 |
34 | The emergence of grammaticality in connectionist networks - Allen, Seidenberg - 1999 |
33 | Can a perceptual processing deficit explain the impairment of inflectional morphology in developmental dysphasia? A computational investigation. - Hoeffner, McClelland - 1993 |
32 | Ambiguity, competition, and blending in spoken word recognition. - Gaskell, Marslen-Wilson - 1999 |
32 | Whammies and double whammies: the effect of length on nonword reading. - Rastle, Coltheart - 1998 |
31 | Reassessing working memory: a comment on Just and Carpenter - MacDonald, Christiansen - 1992 |
28 | A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: extension to sequential processing. - Plaut - 1999 |
23 | Evaluating word-reading models at the item level: matching the grain of theory and data. - Seidenberg, Plaut - 1998 |
23 | Grammar-based connectionist approaches to language. - Smolensky - 1999 |
20 | Bottom-up connectionist modelling of speech. - Cairns - 1995 |
19 | Connectionist sentence processing in perspective. - Steedman - 1999 |
8 | Discriminating local and distributed models of competition in spoken word recognition. - Gaskell, Marslen-Wilson - 1997 |
6 | The role of computational models in neuropsychological investigations of language. Reply to Ruml and Caramazza - Dell - 2000 |
4 | Distributed representation and parallel distributed processing of recursive structures. - Miyata - 1993 |
2 | An evaluation of a computational model of lexical access. - Ruml, Caramazza - 2000 |
2 | Bringing computational models of word recognition down to the item level. - Spieler, Balota - 1997 |
1 | models. - Gaskell, Marslen-Wilson - 1995 |
1 | More words but still no lexicon. - Seidenberg, McClelland - 1990 |