Citations
1625 |
Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data
- Ericsson, Simon
- 1984
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...r the present study, participants were not asked tosprovide explanations or justifications of their thoughts, since verbalizations of that typeshave been argued to be reactive on cognitive processes (=-=Ericsson & Simon, 1993-=-; Leows& Morgan-Short, 2004). The participants were instead instructed simply to verbalizeswhatever thoughts naturally came to mind.sStimulated recall interviews. Another introspective measure, the st... |
1318 |
The study of second language acquisition
- Ellis
- 1994
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s. More specifically, input is likelysto be noticed when it is interpretable (e.g., VanPatten, 1996); meets learners’ expectationssor relates to their L1, L2, or prior knowledge (e.g., Carroll, 1999; =-=Ellis, 1994-=-; Gass, 1997;sPark, 2010); is frequent or perceptually salient (e.g., Bardovi-Harlig, 1987; Ellis, 1994;sGass, 1997; Mackey, 2006); or is meaningful or necessary for understanding (e.g.,sMackey, Gass,... |
355 |
The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition.
- Seifoori, Long, et al.
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...pose deviant learner output with targetlikesformulations. According to Long (2007), “This allows the learner to compare the twosforms side by side, so to speak, and to observe the contrast” (also see =-=Long, 1996-=-; Saxton,s1997). Thirdly, in contrast to other types of interactional feedback such as negotiations,srecasts not only provide negative evidence to alert learners of a mistake, but also supplyspositive... |
186 |
Focus on form: Theory, research, and practice. In
- Long, Robinson
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...t on complexsversions of tasks there will be greater attention to, and uptake of forms madessalient during provision of reactive Focus on Form techniques such as recasts (seesDoughty 2001; Long 2007; =-=Long and Robinson 1998-=-) (Robinson & Gilabert,s2007, p. 167).sIn this statement, they did not make a distinction between resource-dispersing andsresource-directing dimensions regarding the prediction for noticing of interac... |
169 |
Input, interaction, and the second language learner.
- Gass
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...fically, input is likelysto be noticed when it is interpretable (e.g., VanPatten, 1996); meets learners’ expectationssor relates to their L1, L2, or prior knowledge (e.g., Carroll, 1999; Ellis, 1994; =-=Gass, 1997-=-;sPark, 2010); is frequent or perceptually salient (e.g., Bardovi-Harlig, 1987; Ellis, 1994;sGass, 1997; Mackey, 2006); or is meaningful or necessary for understanding (e.g.,sMackey, Gass, & McDonough... |
160 |
Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci.
- Baddeley
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nce learning.sOne cognitive factor that has been receiving a fair amount of attention in SLA issworking memory (WM). WM is “an integrated system for temporarily storing andsmanipulating information” (=-=Baddeley, 2003-=-, p. 837). It underlies learners’ ability toshandle both processing and storing of information simultaneously as they work on aslanguage task; therefore, it has been claimed that learners with higher ... |
116 |
Communicative focus on form.
- Doughty, Varela
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...corrective intentionssappearing quite obvious to learners. This has made researchers argue that the efficacy ofsrecasts on learning may change depending on the type of recasts learners receive (e.g.,s=-=Doughty & Varela, 1998-=-; Han, 2002).sEmpirically, this account regarding the differentiated efficacy of varied types ofsrecasts on development has been supported. Loewen and Philp (2006) revealed thatscharacteristics of rec... |
104 | Choosing and using communication tasks for second language research and instruction. - Pica, Kanagy, et al. - 1993 |
94 |
Conversational interaction and second language development: Recasts, responses, and red herrings?
- Mackey, Philp
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...9), 2) the linguistic target (e.g.,sIwashita, 2003; Leeman, 2003; Mackey, 2006; Ortega & Long, 1997; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,s=-=Mackey & Philp, 1998-=-) or proficiency level (Ammar & Spada, 2006), 4) individualsdifferences (e.g., Goo, 2011; Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, & Tatsumi, 2002; Mackey &sSachs, 2012; Sagarra, 2007; Sheen, 2008; Trofimovich, Amm... |
90 |
Stimulated recall methodology in second language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
- Gass, Mackey
- 2000
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ey, 2000; Mackey, 2012: Mackey, Abbuhl, & Gass, 2012).sIn regard to reactivity, it has been stated that it is less of a concern with stimulatedsrecall protocols than with think-aloud protocols (e.g., =-=Gass & Mackey, 2000-=-). Indeed, itscannot affect performance on the task which participants are recalling because the taskshas already been completed before the recall. However, there have been reports thatsstimulated rec... |
73 |
The influence of planning and task type on second language performance.
- Foster, Skehan
- 1996
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ementation of strategic planning generally produces positive effects on learners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. Crookes, 1989; =-=Foster & Skehan, 1996-=-; Gilabert, 2006; Kawauchi, 2005;s48sMehnert, 1998; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also repo... |
72 |
Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse.
- Lyster
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... their contradicting findings, Loewen and Philpsalternatively speculated that “declarative recasts may be interpreted in the same way as asteacher’s repetition of learners’ TL [targetlike] forms (see =-=Lyster, 1998-=-) and, thus, maysfunction just as implicitly for the learners” (p. 541). Taking into account this explanationsfrom Loewen and Philp, it may be that the intonation of recasts itself does not changesthe... |
67 | Implicit and explicit corrective feedback and the acquisition of l2 grammar. - Ellis, Loewen, et al. - 2006 |
63 | Psychological constraints on the teachability of languages. - Pienemann - 1984 |
60 |
How do learners perceive interactional feedback?
- Mackey, Gass, et al.
- 2000
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...; Ellis, 1994; Gass, 1997;sPark, 2010); is frequent or perceptually salient (e.g., Bardovi-Harlig, 1987; Ellis, 1994;sGass, 1997; Mackey, 2006); or is meaningful or necessary for understanding (e.g.,s=-=Mackey, Gass, & McDonough, 2000-=-; VanPatten, 1996; Mackey, 2006). As far assdevelopmental level is concerned, beginning learners may not notice as much assadvanced learners (Atannassova, 2012); and in addition to the higher developm... |
60 | Attention, memory and the noticing hypothesis - Robinson - 1995 |
56 | Explaining the “natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition” in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants. - Goldschneider, DeKeyser - 2001 |
52 | Input and Interaction. In - Gass - 2003 |
52 |
Output, input enhancement, and the noticing hypothesis: An experimental study on ESL relativization.
- Izumi
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...res, which involve collecting data after learners are exposed to L2 input.sOnline sources of data include eye tracking (Godfroid, Housen, & Boer , 2010), notesstaken by learners (e.g., Hanaoka, 2007; =-=Izumi, 2002-=-), recordings of private speech (e.g.,sOhta, 2000), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., Alanen, 1995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh... |
52 |
Recasts as feedback to language Learners.
- Nicolas, Lightbown, et al.
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...rtly indicate to learners that they have made a mistake. However, it is widelysacknowledged that the degree of implicitness among different types of recasts variessgreatly (e.g., Ellis & Sheen, 2006; =-=Nicholas, Lightbown, & Spada, 2001-=-), and that certainstypes of recasts may be more salient than others, with some recasts’ corrective intentionssappearing quite obvious to learners. This has made researchers argue that the efficacy of... |
51 |
Interlanguage variability in narrative discourse: Style shifting in the use of the past tense.
- Ellis
- 1987
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...more realistic picture of learners’ performance (e.g.,sFoster & Skehan, 1996; Skehan & Foster, 1997), some have focused on the accuracy ofsspecific forms in learners’ production (e.g., Crookes, 1989; =-=Ellis, 1987-=-; Kawauchi, 2005;sMochizuki & Ortega, 2008; Ortega, 1999, 2005; Wigglesworth, 1997). Ortega (1999) hassclaimed, “global measures have the disadvantage of being too broad to capture smallschanges…and o... |
51 | Is language teachable? Psycholinguistic experiments and hypotheses. - Pienemann - 1988 |
50 |
Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior.
- Leow
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...notesstaken by learners (e.g., Hanaoka, 2007; Izumi, 2002), recordings of private speech (e.g.,sOhta, 2000), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., Alanen, 1995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; =-=Leow, 1997-=-, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRobinson, 1997a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), imm... |
45 |
Introduction: Investigating form-focused instruction.
- Ellis
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...y of the data. This concurrency of reporting is an obvioussadvantage; however, it has also been argued to create a risk of reactivity, which maysthreaten the validity of the verbalization data (e.g., =-=Ellis, 2001-=-; Jourdenais, 2001,sRobinson, Mackey, Gass, & Schmidt, 2012). For example, engaging in concurrent verbalsreporting while completing a task may function as an additional task, thereby, imposingsan extr... |
43 |
Planning and Focus on Form
- Ortega
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...earners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996; Gilabert, 2006; Kawauchi, 2005;s48sMehnert, 1998; =-=Ortega, 1999-=-; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also reportedsencouraging results (e.g., complexity: Crookes, 1989; Foste... |
42 |
Input enhancement and rule presentation in second language Acquisition. In
- Alanen
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...a include eye tracking (Godfroid, Housen, & Boer , 2010), notesstaken by learners (e.g., Hanaoka, 2007; Izumi, 2002), recordings of private speech (e.g.,sOhta, 2000), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., =-=Alanen, 1995-=-; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRob... |
42 |
Corrective feedback and learner uptake.
- Lyster, Ranta
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...2 development” (p. 182).sRecasts have been found to be the most frequently used type of interactionalsfeedback in instructional settings (e.g., Ellis, Basturkmen, & Loewen, 2001; Lyster &sMori, 2006; =-=Lyster & Ranta, 1997-=-). Gass and Mackey (2007) define a recast as “asrephrasing of an incorrect utterance using a correct form while maintaining the originalsmeaning” (p. 182) (see Example 1). One of recasts’ unique and h... |
41 |
Planning and interlanguage variation.
- Crookes
- 1989
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...n that thesimplementation of strategic planning generally produces positive effects on learners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. =-=Crookes, 1989-=-; Foster & Skehan, 1996; Gilabert, 2006; Kawauchi, 2005;s48sMehnert, 1998; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies,... |
37 |
A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behavior: Aware vs. unaware learners.
- Leow
- 2000
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... the issue of awareness.sSummary on NoticingsThe studies with more sound methodologies as far as the measurement ofsawareness is concerned (Faretta-Stutenberg & Morgan-Short, 2011; Hama & Leow, 2010;s=-=Leow, 2000-=-) consistently suggested that noticing is necessary for L2 learning, as Schmidtsand Robinson claimed (e.g., Robinson, 2003; Schmidt, 2001); however, furthersexamination is undoubtedly essential in ord... |
36 |
The effects of different lengths of time for planning on second language performance.
- Mehnert
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ve effects on learners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996; Gilabert, 2006; Kawauchi, 2005;s48s=-=Mehnert, 1998-=-; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also reportedsencouraging results (e.g., complexity: Crooke... |
34 |
Factors influencing the development of language proficiency
- Pienemann, Johnston
- 1987
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s. Philp (2003) assigned her participants into high-,sintermediate- and low-level groups based on their production of question forms accordingsto the six stages of development for question formation (=-=Pienemann & Johnston, 1987-=-).s41sPhilp operationalized noticing as the accurate immediate recall of recasts. During aspicture drawing task, learners asked a native interlocutor questions to complete thespicture drawing and the ... |
32 |
Recasts and second language development: Beyond negative evidence.
- Leeman
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...sthe efficacy of recasts on L2 development. 1sThese include, but are not limited to: 1) thestype of recast (e.g., Loewen & Philp, 2006; Nassaji, 2009), 2) the linguistic target (e.g.,sIwashita, 2003; =-=Leeman, 2003-=-; Mackey, 2006; Ortega & Long, 1997; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,sMackey & Philp, 1998) or proficiency level (Ammar & Spada, 2006)... |
32 |
Constraints on ‘‘noticing the gap’’: non-native speakers’ noticing of recasts
- Philp
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...osa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRobinson, 1997a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), immediate recall (=-=Philp, 2003-=-),simmediate reports (Egi, 2004), and stimulated recall protocols (e.g., Adams, 2003; Egi,s2010; Mackey, 2006; Mackey et al., 2000; Nabei & Swain, 2002; Swain & Lapkin, 2002).sAmong those various meas... |
30 | Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. - Mackey, Goo - 2007 |
28 | Interactional feedback and instructional counterbalance - Lyster, Mori - 2006 |
28 |
Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning.
- Robinson
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,s=-=Robinson, 1997-=-a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), immediate recall (Philp, 2003),simmediate reports (Egi, 2004), and stimulated recall protocols (e.g., Adams, 2003; Egi,s2010; Mackey, 2006; Mackey et al., 200... |
27 |
One size fits all? Recasts, prompts
- Ammar, Spada
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ..., 2003; Leeman, 2003; Mackey, 2006; Ortega & Long, 1997; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,sMackey & Philp, 1998) or proficiency level (=-=Ammar & Spada, 2006-=-), 4) individualsdifferences (e.g., Goo, 2011; Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, & Tatsumi, 2002; Mackey &sSachs, 2012; Sagarra, 2007; Sheen, 2008; Trofimovich, Ammar, & Gatbonton, 2007),sand 5) task variabl... |
27 |
Feedback, noticing and instructed second language learning.
- Mackey
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nssor relates to their L1, L2, or prior knowledge (e.g., Carroll, 1999; Ellis, 1994; Gass, 1997;sPark, 2010); is frequent or perceptually salient (e.g., Bardovi-Harlig, 1987; Ellis, 1994;sGass, 1997; =-=Mackey, 2006-=-); or is meaningful or necessary for understanding (e.g.,sMackey, Gass, & McDonough, 2000; VanPatten, 1996; Mackey, 2006). As far assdevelopmental level is concerned, beginning learners may not notice... |
26 |
The influence of source of planning and focus of planning on task-based performance.
- Foster, Skehan
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...e evaluated whethersproviding detailed suggestions regarding the use of strategic planning time as part of thesplanning instructions would positively impact learners’ production. Other studies (e.g.,s=-=Foster & Skehan, 1999-=-; Mochizuki & Ortega, 2008; Sangarun, 2005) have examinedswhether content-focused or grammar-focused instructions would change learners’sproduction. In the studies by Foster and Skehan (1996) investig... |
26 | Attention as a cognitive and neural system. - Posner - 1992 |
25 |
What concept of consciousness?
- Allport
- 1988
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ndstheir ability to provide protocols must be considered” (p. 374). It has also been cautionedsthat the lack of verbal report cannot be taken as evidence that learners did not noticessomething (e.g., =-=Allport, 1988-=-; Mackey et al., 2000; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999). In manysstudies, therefore, in order to increase the robustness of their data, researchers acquirestriangulated data by additionally utilizing some type o... |
24 | Input, interaction, and output in second language acquisition. - GASS, MACKEY - 2015 |
24 |
A study of the impact of recasts on tense consistency in L2 output.
- Han
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...pearing quite obvious to learners. This has made researchers argue that the efficacy ofsrecasts on learning may change depending on the type of recasts learners receive (e.g.,sDoughty & Varela, 1998; =-=Han, 2002-=-).sEmpirically, this account regarding the differentiated efficacy of varied types ofsrecasts on development has been supported. Loewen and Philp (2006) revealed thatscharacteristics of recasts such a... |
24 |
Individual differences in working memory, noticing of interactional feedback and L2 development. In
- Mackey, Philp, et al.
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...y work on aslanguage task; therefore, it has been claimed that learners with higher WM may receives34smore benefit from recasts. A number of researchers have empirically tested this claims(Goo, 2011; =-=Mackey et al., 2002-=-; Mackey & Sachs, 2012; Révész, 2012; Sagarra, 2007;sTrofimovich et al., 2007) and have revealed somewhat mixed results.sRévész (2012), Sagarra (2007), and Mackey and Sachs (2012) found the expectedsa... |
22 | Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences: Applications to syntax. - Carr, Curren - 1994 |
21 | To think aloud or not to think aloud: he issue of reactivity in SLA research methodology. - Leow, Morgan-Short - 2004 |
21 |
Rethinking recasts: a learner-centered examination of corrective feedback in the Japanese language classroom. In
- Ohta
- 2000
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...are exposed to L2 input.sOnline sources of data include eye tracking (Godfroid, Housen, & Boer , 2010), notesstaken by learners (e.g., Hanaoka, 2007; Izumi, 2002), recordings of private speech (e.g.,s=-=Ohta, 2000-=-), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., Alanen, 1995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., S... |
19 | Do learners notice enhanced forms while interacting with the L2? An online and off-line study of the role of written input enhancement in L2 reading. - Leow - 2001 |
18 |
The differential effects of three types of task planning on the fluency, complexity, and accuracy in L2 oral production.
- Ellis
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...er deliberately or non-deliberately, on items the task instructions do notsspecify. Many researchers have stressed the need for more studies of what learners dosduring planning (e.g., Batstone, 2005; =-=Ellis, 2009-=-; Mochizuki & Ortega, 2008; Ortega,s1999, 2005; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 2005). However, thus far, few (Ortega,s1995, 2005; Sangarun, 2005) have explored this area.sLearners’ perceptions. Just... |
16 |
Awareness and the efficacy of error correction. In
- Roberts
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...h as stimulated recall (Egi, 2007a; Gass & Lewis, 2007; Kim & Han, 2007;sMackey, 2006; Mackey, Al-Khalil, Atanassova, Hama, Logan-Terry, & Nakatsukasa,s2007; Mackey et al., 2002; Nabei & Swain, 2002; =-=Roberts, 1995-=-), cued immediate recalls(Philp, 2003), immediate report (Egi, 2007a), and private speech (Ohta, 2000). Thesesstudies have revealed that learners do, indeed, notice the language in recasts. Moreover,s... |
14 |
he efects of strategic planning on the oral narratives of learners with low and high intermediate levels of L2 proiciency. In
- Kawauchi
- 2005
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...lly produces positive effects on learners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996; Gilabert, 2006; =-=Kawauchi, 2005-=-;s48sMehnert, 1998; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also reportedsencouraging results (e.g., ... |
13 | Interpreting recasts as linguistic evidence: The roles of linguistic target, length, and degree of change. - Egi - 2007 |
13 | Uptake, modified output, and learner perceptions of recasts: Learner responses as language awareness. The Modern Language - Egi - 2010 |
12 |
L2 output, reformulation and noticing: implications for IL development
- Adams
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...idt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRobinson, 1997a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), immediate recall (Philp, 2003),simmediate reports (Egi, 2004), and stimulated recall protocols (e.g., =-=Adams, 2003-=-; Egi,s2010; Mackey, 2006; Mackey et al., 2000; Nabei & Swain, 2002; Swain & Lapkin, 2002).sAmong those various measures, two types of verbal protocols, think-aloud and stimulatedsrecall, have been th... |
11 |
The think-aloud controversy in second language research
- Bowles
- 2010
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s on observing learners’ cognitive processes during oral communicativesactivities because of the impossibility of engaging in conversation while simultaneouslysverbalizing one’s inner thoughts (e.g., =-=Bowles, 2010-=-; Jourdenais, 2001) and stimulatedsrecall protocols have been a preferred method of data collection. In stimulated recallsprotocols, learners first engage in L2 activity. These learners’ task performa... |
11 | What do learners plan?: Learner-driven attention to from during pre-task planning - Ortega |
11 |
The effects of models and recasts on the acquisition of object topicalization and adverb placement in L2 Spanish.
- Ortega, Long
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...L2 development. 1sThese include, but are not limited to: 1) thestype of recast (e.g., Loewen & Philp, 2006; Nassaji, 2009), 2) the linguistic target (e.g.,sIwashita, 2003; Leeman, 2003; Mackey, 2006; =-=Ortega & Long, 1997-=-; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,sMackey & Philp, 1998) or proficiency level (Ammar & Spada, 2006), 4) individualsdifferences (e.g., ... |
10 |
The simultaneous manipulation of task complexity along planning time and +/−Here-and-Now: Effects on oral production
- Gilabert
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... planning generally produces positive effects on learners’ tasksperformance. The studies have revealed clear and consistent confirmatory effects onsfluency (e.g. Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996; =-=Gilabert, 2006-=-; Kawauchi, 2005;s48sMehnert, 1998; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005). Forscomplexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also reportedsencouraging... |
10 |
Recasts in communicative EFL classes: Do teacher intent and learner interpretation overlap? In
- Kim, Han
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...Planning, Noticing, and L2 Development.s39slearners’ noticing when they were given recasts have been undertaken using introspectivesmeasures such as stimulated recall (Egi, 2007a; Gass & Lewis, 2007; =-=Kim & Han, 2007-=-;sMackey, 2006; Mackey, Al-Khalil, Atanassova, Hama, Logan-Terry, & Nakatsukasa,s2007; Mackey et al., 2002; Nabei & Swain, 2002; Roberts, 1995), cued immediate recalls(Philp, 2003), immediate report (... |
10 | The implicit learning of mappings between forms and contextually derived meanings. - Leung, Williams - 2011 |
10 |
Balancing communication and grammar in beginninglevel foreign language classrooms: A study of guided planning and relativization.
- Mochizuki, Ortega
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...lexity and accuracy, with the exception of a few studies, research has also reportedsencouraging results (e.g., complexity: Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996, 1999;sGilabert, 2006; Kawauchi, 2005; =-=Mochizuki & Ortega, 2008-=-; Ortega, 1999; Sangarun,s2005; Skehan & Foster, 1997, 2005; Tavakoli & Skehan, 2005; Wigglesworth, 1997;sYuan & Ellis, 2003; accuracy: Crookes, 1989; Foster & Skehan, 1996, 1999; Kawauchi,s2005; Mehn... |
9 | The Acquisition of Japanese. D. Slobin (ed), The Crosslinguislic Study of Language Acquisition. Flillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates - Clancy - 1985 |
9 |
Recasts in SLA: The story so far. In
- Long
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nd at the same time, they also generate opportunities for learners to notice newsinformation via their positive evidence. In other words, recasts can directly assist learnerssto create new knowledge (=-=Long, 2007-=-).s28sRecasts and L2 Development: Empirical EvidencesSince the mid-1990s, the link between recasts and L2 development has beensextensively tested, and the results from those studies have largely demon... |
9 | Effects of recasts and elicitations in dyadic interaction and the role of feedback explicitness. - Nassaji - 2009 |
8 |
Re-examining the Role of Recasts
- Ellis, Sheen
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...8) which does notsovertly indicate to learners that they have made a mistake. However, it is widelysacknowledged that the degree of implicitness among different types of recasts variessgreatly (e.g., =-=Ellis & Sheen, 2006-=-; Nicholas, Lightbown, & Spada, 2001), and that certainstypes of recasts may be more salient than others, with some recasts’ corrective intentionssappearing quite obvious to learners. This has made re... |
7 |
Recasts in the adult L2 classroom: characteristics, explicitness and effectiveness
- Loewen, Philp
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...12).sResearchers, so far, have found empirical evidence that several factors influencesthe efficacy of recasts on L2 development. 1sThese include, but are not limited to: 1) thestype of recast (e.g., =-=Loewen & Philp, 2006-=-; Nassaji, 2009), 2) the linguistic target (e.g.,sIwashita, 2003; Leeman, 2003; Mackey, 2006; Ortega & Long, 1997; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental rea... |
6 | Task complexity, the Cognition Hypothesis and interaction in CMC and FTF environments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation - Baralt - 2010 |
6 |
Output, noticing, and learning: An investigation into the role of spontaneous attention to form in a four-stage writing task,”
- Hanaoka
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...) offline measures, which involve collecting data after learners are exposed to L2 input.sOnline sources of data include eye tracking (Godfroid, Housen, & Boer , 2010), notesstaken by learners (e.g., =-=Hanaoka, 2007-=-; Izumi, 2002), recordings of private speech (e.g.,sOhta, 2000), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., Alanen, 1995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999... |
6 |
Teachers' intentions and learners' perceptions about corrective feedback on the L2 classroom.
- Mackey, Al-Khalil, et al.
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... results in regards to the intonation of recasts (i.e.,sdeclarative vs. interrogative) have depicted a mixed pattern; specifically, more noticings40swas found with interrogative recasts in one study (=-=Mackey et al., 2007-=-), but the oppositesfindings were revealed in the other (Kim & Han, 2007). This contradicting patternssupports the conclusion drawn from Loewen and Philp’s (2006) developmental study,sbased on which t... |
6 |
Task complexity, focus on form, and second language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition
- Révész
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...) individualsdifferences (e.g., Goo, 2011; Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, & Tatsumi, 2002; Mackey &sSachs, 2012; Sagarra, 2007; Sheen, 2008; Trofimovich, Ammar, & Gatbonton, 2007),sand 5) task variables (=-=Révész, 2009-=-; Révész & Han, 2006; Romanova, 2010). Below, Iswill briefly describe claims which have been made for each factor and the empiricalsfindings from previous studies (see also Mackey, 2012).s1sThe result... |
5 |
Verbal report, noticing
- Egi
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRobinson, 1997a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), immediate recall (Philp, 2003),simmediate reports (=-=Egi, 2004-=-), and stimulated recall protocols (e.g., Adams, 2003; Egi,s2010; Mackey, 2006; Mackey et al., 2000; Nabei & Swain, 2002; Swain & Lapkin, 2002).sAmong those various measures, two types of verbal proto... |
5 |
Planning for test performance: Does it make a difference
- Elder, Iwashita
- 2005
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ralt (2010),sfor example, found that learners rated a cognitively more complex task as being moresdifficult.sAt the same time, though, several studies investigating the effects of strategicsplanning (=-=Elder & Iwashita, 2005-=-; Tavakoli & Skehan, 2005; Wigglesworth, 2001) havesreported mixed results regarding learners’ perceptions of task difficulty. Tavakoli andsSkehan (2005) found that planners perceived tasks to be sign... |
5 | A procedure for testing the noticing hypothesis in the context of vocabulary acquisition - Godfroid, Housen, et al. - 2010 |
5 |
Models, attention, and awareness in SLA: A response to Simard and Wong's "Alertness, orientation, and detection: The conceptualization of attentional functions
- Leow
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context .... This may occur because during thesrecall session, learners are exposed to the same L2 input they experienced in the initialsactivity; therefore, it creates a situation called double input-exposure (=-=Leow, 2002-=-). Thissmeans that learners have an additional opportunity for processing input which they mayshave not been able to process during the initial exposure. Additionally, this doublesexposure may possibl... |
4 |
Planning and task-based research: theory and research.
- Ellis
- 2005
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...in this area to incorporate standardsexperimental designs involving a pre-test, a treatment, and posttests.sIn addition to these concerns with demonstrating L2 development, severalsresearchers (e.g., =-=Ellis, 2005-=-; Mackey, 2012; Robinson, 2001; Robinson & Gilabert, 2007)shave also expressed that future task planning studies must extend the scope of their focussto include the actual processes learners go throug... |
4 |
Task content familiarity, task type, and efficacy of recasts. Language Awareness
- Révész, Han
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ifferences (e.g., Goo, 2011; Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, & Tatsumi, 2002; Mackey &sSachs, 2012; Sagarra, 2007; Sheen, 2008; Trofimovich, Ammar, & Gatbonton, 2007),sand 5) task variables (Révész, 2009; =-=Révész & Han, 2006-=-; Romanova, 2010). Below, Iswill briefly describe claims which have been made for each factor and the empiricalsfindings from previous studies (see also Mackey, 2012).s1sThe results from studies utili... |
3 |
Learning without awareness revisited: Extending
- Hama, Leow
- 2010
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...tracking (Godfroid, Housen, & Boer , 2010), notesstaken by learners (e.g., Hanaoka, 2007; Izumi, 2002), recordings of private speech (e.g.,sOhta, 2000), and think-aloud protocols (e.g., Alanen, 1995; =-=Hama & Leow, 2010-=-;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,sRobinson, 1997a, b), i... |
3 |
Protocol analysis and SLA
- Jourdenais
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context .... This concurrency of reporting is an obvioussadvantage; however, it has also been argued to create a risk of reactivity, which maysthreaten the validity of the verbalization data (e.g., Ellis, 2001; =-=Jourdenais, 2001-=-,sRobinson, Mackey, Gass, & Schmidt, 2012). For example, engaging in concurrent verbalsreporting while completing a task may function as an additional task, thereby, imposingsan extra processing load,... |
3 |
Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory
- Mackey, Sachs
- 2012
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...task; therefore, it has been claimed that learners with higher WM may receives34smore benefit from recasts. A number of researchers have empirically tested this claims(Goo, 2011; Mackey et al., 2002; =-=Mackey & Sachs, 2012-=-; Révész, 2012; Sagarra, 2007;sTrofimovich et al., 2007) and have revealed somewhat mixed results.sRévész (2012), Sagarra (2007), and Mackey and Sachs (2012) found the expectedsassociation between WM ... |
3 |
Automaticity and generalizability of second language learning under implicit, incidental, enhanced and rule-search conditions.
- Robinson
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...995; Hama & Leow, 2010;sJourdenais et al., 1995; Leow, 1997, 2000, 2001; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh,s2007). Offline sources include diaries (e.g., Schmidt & Frota, 1986), questionnaires (e.g.,s=-=Robinson, 1997-=-a, b), interviews (e.g., Williams, 2005), immediate recall (Philp, 2003),simmediate reports (Egi, 2004), and stimulated recall protocols (e.g., Adams, 2003; Egi,s2010; Mackey, 2006; Mackey et al., 200... |
2 |
Investigating stimulated recall as a cognitive measure: Reactivity and verbal reports in SLA research methodology
- Egi
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...dy been completed before the recall. However, there have been reports thatsstimulated recall may be reactive on tasks performed following the recall session (e.g.,sAdams, 2003; Egi, 2004 however, see =-=Egi, 2008-=-). This may occur because during thesrecall session, learners are exposed to the same L2 input they experienced in the initialsactivity; therefore, it creates a situation called double input-exposure ... |
2 | Attention as a psychological entity and its effects on language and Communication - Eviatar - 1998 |
2 |
Perceptions of interactional feedback: Differences between heritage language learners and non-heritage language learners
- Gass, Lewis
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... chapter, Strategic Planning, Noticing, and L2 Development.s39slearners’ noticing when they were given recasts have been undertaken using introspectivesmeasures such as stimulated recall (Egi, 2007a; =-=Gass & Lewis, 2007-=-; Kim & Han, 2007;sMackey, 2006; Mackey, Al-Khalil, Atanassova, Hama, Logan-Terry, & Nakatsukasa,s2007; Mackey et al., 2002; Nabei & Swain, 2002; Roberts, 1995), cued immediate recalls(Philp, 2003), i... |
2 |
Corrective feedback, individual variation in cognitive capacities, and L2 development: Recasts vs. metalinguistic feedback. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. (Accession Order No
- Goo
- 2011
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,sMackey & Philp, 1998) or proficiency level (Ammar & Spada, 2006), 4) individualsdifferences (e.g., =-=Goo, 2011-=-; Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, & Tatsumi, 2002; Mackey &sSachs, 2012; Sagarra, 2007; Sheen, 2008; Trofimovich, Ammar, & Gatbonton, 2007),sand 5) task variables (Révész, 2009; Révész & Han, 2006; Romanov... |
2 | Explicit memory for unattended information - Kellogg, Dare - 1989 |
2 |
Input, interaction and corrective feedback in L2 learning
- Mackey
- 2012
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ask variables (Révész, 2009; Révész & Han, 2006; Romanova, 2010). Below, Iswill briefly describe claims which have been made for each factor and the empiricalsfindings from previous studies (see also =-=Mackey, 2012-=-).s1sThe results from studies utilizing written recasts are not included here. This is indeed one of the areas thatsinteraction researchers should investigate, and their results have provided and will... |
2 |
Epilogue: Exploring the intricacies of interaction and language development
- Philp
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... have started exploring one of the theoretical premises ofsrecasts--the theory that recasts may lead learners to notice their utterances aresnontargetlike and/or specific nontargetlike aspects (e.g., =-=Philp, 2009-=-). Examinations ofs3sHere I refrained from explaining how the two models of attention, a single-resource model and a multipleresource model, explain the results of the two studies by Révész. This is b... |
1 | Second language motivation: Its relationship to noticing, affect, and production in task-based interaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation - Al-Khalil - 2011 |
1 | Beginning and advanced learners' awareness of corrective feedback in the Arabic foreign language classroom. Unpublished doctoral dissertation - Atanassova - 2012 |
1 | Pragmatics in second language - Bardovi-Harlig - 2012 |
1 |
Planning as discourse activity. In R. Ellis (Ed.), Planning and task performance in a second language
- Batstone
- 2005
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...smay focus, either deliberately or non-deliberately, on items the task instructions do notsspecify. Many researchers have stressed the need for more studies of what learners dosduring planning (e.g., =-=Batstone, 2005-=-; Ellis, 2009; Mochizuki & Ortega, 2008; Ortega,s1999, 2005; Sangarun, 2005; Skehan & Foster, 2005). However, thus far, few (Ortega,s1995, 2005; Sangarun, 2005) have explored this area.sLearners’ perc... |
1 |
It's vocabulary"/It's gender": Learner awareness and incidental learning. Language Awareness
- Bell, Collins
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... the more learning they demonstrated as well (e.g., Alanen, 1995; Leow, 1997,s2000, 2001ab; Mackey, 2006; Rosa & Leow, 2004; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Polio,s2007; Sachs & Suh, 2007; however, see =-=Bell & Collins, 2009-=-). The results from thosesstudies emphasize the fact that awareness, regardless of its necessity, provides a verysstrong association with SLA.sIn the following section, I will describe theoretical arg... |
1 | learners’ perceptions, and L2 development. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A series of empirical studies (pp - Egi |
1 |
Learner uptake in communicative classrooms
- Ellis, Basturkmen, et al.
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s on their production or comprehension, thus promoting L2 development” (p. 182).sRecasts have been found to be the most frequently used type of interactionalsfeedback in instructional settings (e.g., =-=Ellis, Basturkmen, & Loewen, 2001-=-; Lyster &sMori, 2006; Lyster & Ranta, 1997). Gass and Mackey (2007) define a recast as “asrephrasing of an incorrect utterance using a correct form while maintaining the originalsmeaning” (p. 182) (s... |
1 |
Learning without awareness: Williams (2005) reconsidered
- Faretta-Stutenberg, Morgan-Short
- 2011
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...fore, it is essential to triangulatesdata when investigating the issue of awareness.sSummary on NoticingsThe studies with more sound methodologies as far as the measurement ofsawareness is concerned (=-=Faretta-Stutenberg & Morgan-Short, 2011-=-; Hama & Leow, 2010;sLeow, 2000) consistently suggested that noticing is necessary for L2 learning, as Schmidtsand Robinson claimed (e.g., Robinson, 2003; Schmidt, 2001); however, furthersexamination ... |
1 | Nenshouji no jukyuu hyougen. In F. Peng & M. Hori (Eds.), Kotoba no hattatsu: Brain function and language acquisition (pp. 51-76). Hiroshima: Bunka Hyouron Publishing Co - Horiguchi - 1979 |
1 |
Positive and negative input in task-based interaction: differential affects
- Iwashita
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...actors influencesthe efficacy of recasts on L2 development. 1sThese include, but are not limited to: 1) thestype of recast (e.g., Loewen & Philp, 2006; Nassaji, 2009), 2) the linguistic target (e.g.,s=-=Iwashita, 2003-=-; Leeman, 2003; Mackey, 2006; Ortega & Long, 1997; Yang & Lyster,s2010; Yilmaz & Yuksel, 2011; Witzel & Ono, 2003), 3) developmental readiness (e.g.,sMackey & Philp, 1998) or proficiency level (Ammar ... |
1 | Does textual enhancement promote noticing? A think-aloud protocol analysis - unknown authors - 1995 |
1 |
Interactionist approaches
- Mackey, Abbuhl, et al.
- 2012
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...s13saudio-recordings as stimuli to serve as memory aids and assist with recall. This allowssresearchers to obtain more accurate and complete responses (Bloom, 1954; Gass &sMackey, 2000; Mackey, 2012: =-=Mackey, Abbuhl, & Gass, 2012-=-).sIn regard to reactivity, it has been stated that it is less of a concern with stimulatedsrecall protocols than with think-aloud protocols (e.g., Gass & Mackey, 2000). Indeed, itscannot affect perfo... |
1 | Measuring cognitive task demands: A step forward in task complexity research. EUROSLA 22 [Conference - Révész, Michel, et al. - 2012 |