DMCA
The motivation–cognition interface in learning and decision making (2010)
Venue: | Current Directions in Psychological Science |
Citations: | 13 - 5 self |
Citations
775 |
Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans.
- Steele, Aronson
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...onal state need not be induced by an explicit reward. For example, research on stereotype threat documents performance decrements resulting from the activation of a negative task-relevant stereotype (=-=Steele & Aronson, 1995-=-; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002; Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999). For example, there is a pervasive stereotype in the United States that women are worse at math than men, and that stereoty... |
526 | Beyond pleasure and pain. - Higgins - 1997 |
500 | Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey
- Locke, Latham
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...offering some global positive incentive for good performance, such as a promotion, a bonus, or praise, that is meant to boost interest in the task or increase a person’s sense of self-efficacy (e.g., =-=Locke & Latham, 2002-=-). How many parents have offered monetary incentives for grades ($10 for each B, $20 for each A, etc.) in hopes of motivating their teenager to maximize his or her GPA? A Framework for the Motivation–... |
182 |
Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat.
- Steele, Spencer, et al.
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nduced by an explicit reward. For example, research on stereotype threat documents performance decrements resulting from the activation of a negative task-relevant stereotype (Steele & Aronson, 1995; =-=Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002-=-; Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999). For example, there is a pervasive stereotype in the United States that women are worse at math than men, and that stereotype leads women to perform worse th... |
181 | Comparing decision bound and exemplar models of categorization.
- Maddox, Ashby
- 1993
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...er, Grimm, Rein, & Maddox, 2009). Critically, this involves the application of formal modeling techniques that allow the researcher to dissect the task and isolate the underlying cognitive processes (=-=Maddox & Ashby, 1993-=-, 2004). Using this approach, we find that there is a three-way interaction between global incentives, local incentives, and the task demands associated with optimizing performance that determines the... |
103 |
A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task.
- Busemeyer, Stout
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... the effects of motivation on cognition. The next step will be to formalize our motivational framework by investigating components of currently successful models such as the expectancy valence model (=-=Busemeyer & Stout, 2002-=-). A second avenue is to take a cognitive neuroscience approach to the motivation–cognition interface by attempting to identify the neural systems associated with a match or a mismatch. One intriguing... |
98 | Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance.
- Stone, Lynch, et al.
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...example, research on stereotype threat documents performance decrements resulting from the activation of a negative task-relevant stereotype (Steele & Aronson, 1995; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002; =-=Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999-=-). For example, there is a pervasive stereotype in the United States that women are worse at math than men, and that stereotype leads women to perform worse than men of equal ability on math assessmen... |
93 | Dissociating explicit and procedural-learning based systems of perceptual category learning. - Maddox, Ashby - 2004 |
59 | Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci.
- Seibt, Förster
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...sted that stereotype threat can be reinterpreted as a mismatch between global and local incentives by drawing upon prior work suggesting that negative stereotypes induce a global avoidance incentive (=-=Seibt & Forster, 2004-=-). If this is correct, then stereotype-threat effects may reflect that those with a negative self-relevant stereotype are experiencing a mismatch between a global incentive (trying to avoid confirming... |
38 | A test of the regulatory fit hypothesis in perceptual classification learning.
- Maddox, Baldwin, et al.
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...n our research, summarized in Table 1, is that a ‘‘match’’ between the global incentive and the local incentive leads to more flexible and exploratory cognitive processing than does a mismatch (e.g., =-=Maddox, Baldwin, & Markman, 2006-=-). We illustrate this idea with two simple examples, both from a widget factory. In the first example (Table 2), imagine an employee in the Research and Development office at a widget factory, who has... |
23 | Regulatory fit effects in a choice task - Worthy, Maddox, et al. - 2007 |
21 | Stereotype threat reinterpreted as a regulatory mismatch. - Grimm, Markman, et al. - 2009 |
18 |
Differential Effects of Regulatory Fit on Category Learning
- Grimm, Markman, et al.
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...bal and local incentives leads to faster informationintegration learning than does a mismatch, because a mismatch leads to a decrease in cognitive flexibility and thus less explicit rule processing. (=-=Grimm, Markman, Maddox, & Baldwin, 2008-=-; Maddox et al., 2006). We illustrate this pattern of results in Figure 1. Motivation and Choice In the domain of choice, Worthy, Maddox, and Markman (2007) found a similar pattern of results. In this... |
17 |
The neuropsychology of impulsive antisocial sensation seeking personality traits: from dopamine to hippocampal function
- Pickering
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...onal states between patients and controls rather than differences in the capacity to think. Permanent trait (personality) variables, such as impulsivity, have clear motivational effects on cognition (=-=Pickering, 2004-=-). There are a number of research challenges ahead. The primary challenge is to understand why this complex interaction is observed. It is clear that the influence of a match between the global and lo... |
4 |
The optimal level of fuzz: Case studies in a methodology for psychological research
- Markman, Beer, et al.
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...work for the Motivation–Cognition Interface Our research assumes that studying the influence of incentives on thinking requires experimental tasks for which the cognitive demands are well understood (=-=Markman, Beer, Grimm, Rein, & Maddox, 2009-=-). Critically, this involves the application of formal modeling techniques that allow the researcher to dissect the task and isolate the underlying cognitive processes (Maddox & Ashby, 1993, 2004). Us... |