• Documents
  • Authors
  • Tables
  • Log in
  • Sign up
  • MetaCart
  • DMCA
  • Donate

CiteSeerX logo

Advanced Search Include Citations
Advanced Search Include Citations | Disambiguate

Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. (2000)

by L E James, D M Burke
Add To MetaCart

Tools

Sorted by:
Results 1 - 10 of 34
Next 10 →

The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults

by Michael S. Vitevitch - Memory & Cognition , 2003
"... The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on the speed and accuracy of speech production was investigated with speech-error elicitation and picture-naming tasks. The results from 2 speech-error elicitation techniques—the spoonerisms of laboratory induced predisposition technique (B. J. ..."
Abstract - Cited by 72 (3 self) - Add to MetaCart
The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on the speed and accuracy of speech production was investigated with speech-error elicitation and picture-naming tasks. The results from 2 speech-error elicitation techniques—the spoonerisms of laboratory induced predisposition technique (B. J. Baars,
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...result of word forms and phonological segments interacting with each another. Although the results and conclusions of this set of experiments may appear incongruent with those of other studies (e.g., =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-; Sevald & Dell, 1994; Yaniv, Meyer, Gordon, Huff, & Sevald, 1990), they, in fact, are consistent within a broader view of the speech-production system. For example, James and Burke (2000) found fewer...

Aging and language production

by Deborah M. Burke, Meredith A. Shafto - Current Directions in Psychological Science , 2004
"... ABSTRACT—Experimental research and older adults ’ reports of their own experience suggest that the ability to produce the spoken forms of familiar words declines with aging. Older adults experience more word-finding failures, such as tip-of-thetongue states, than young adults do, and this and other ..."
Abstract - Cited by 22 (3 self) - Add to MetaCart
ABSTRACT—Experimental research and older adults ’ reports of their own experience suggest that the ability to produce the spoken forms of familiar words declines with aging. Older adults experience more word-finding failures, such as tip-of-thetongue states, than young adults do, and this and other speech production failures appear to stem from difficulties in retrieving the sounds of words. Recent evidence has identified a parallel age-related decline in retrieving the spelling of familiar words. Models of cognitive aging must explain why these aspects of language production decline with aging whereas semantic processes are well maintained. We describe a model wherein aging weakens connections among linguistic representations, thereby reducing the transmission of excitation from one representation to another. The structure of the representational systems for word phonology and orthography makes them vulnerable to
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...rget word will reduce subsequent TOTs for the target word. This is because production of the phonologically related words strengthens weak phonological connections that cause TOTs. In one study (L.E. =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-), participants were asked a series of TOT-inducing questions, and whenever they responded ‘‘TOT’’ or ‘‘don’t know,’’ they were given 10 words to pronounce before the question was presented a second t...

On the tipof-the-tongue: Neural correlates of increased word-finding failures in normal aging

by Meredith A Shafto , Deborah M Burke , Emmanuel A Stamatakis , Phyllis P Tam , Lorraine K Tyler - Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 2007
"... Abstract & Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) experiences are frustrating wordfinding failures where people are temporarily unable to produce a word they are certain they know. TOT frequency increases with normal aging during adulthood, and behavioral evidence suggests that the underlying deficit is in re ..."
Abstract - Cited by 15 (2 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract & Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) experiences are frustrating wordfinding failures where people are temporarily unable to produce a word they are certain they know. TOT frequency increases with normal aging during adulthood, and behavioral evidence suggests that the underlying deficit is in retrieving the complete phonology of the target word during production. The present study investigated the neural correlates of this phonological retrieval deficit. We obtained 3-D T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance images (MRI) for healthy participants between 19 and 88 years old and used voxel-based morphometry to measure gray matter density throughout the brain. In a separate session, participants named celebrities cued by pictures and descriptions, indicating when they had a TOT, and also completed Raven's Progressive Matrices (RPM), a task that does not involve phonological production. The number of TOTs increased with age and also with gray matter atrophy in the left insula, an area implicated in phonological production. The relation between TOTs and left insula atrophy cannot be attributed to the correlation of each variable with age because TOTs were related to insula atrophy even with age effects removed. Moreover, errors on the RPM increased with age, but performance did not correlate with gray matter density in the insula. These results provide, for the first time, an association between a region in the neural language system and the rise in age-related word-finding failures and suggest that age-related atrophy in neural regions important for phonological production may contribute to age-related word production failures. &
(Show Context)

Citation Context

... sound segments that overlapped some of the sounds of the target word (e.g., abdicate). Pronouncing prime words increased resolution of the TOT when the definition was repeated (White & Abrams, 2002; =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-). Similar effects were observed when participants produced a homophone of a proper name in response to a definition (e.g., The hard stone, as of the plum or cherry, which contains the seed is called ...

Asymmetric aging effects on semantic and phonological processes: Naming in the picture-word interference task

by Jennifer K Taylor , Deborah M Burke - Neuropsychology , 2002
"... In 2 experiments, participants named pictures while ignoring auditory word distractors. For pictures with homophone names (e.g., ball), distractors semantically related to the nondepicted meaning (e.g., prom) facilitated naming by top-down phonological connections for young but not for older adults ..."
Abstract - Cited by 13 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
In 2 experiments, participants named pictures while ignoring auditory word distractors. For pictures with homophone names (e.g., ball), distractors semantically related to the nondepicted meaning (e.g., prom) facilitated naming by top-down phonological connections for young but not for older adults. Slowing from unrelated distractors and facilitation from phonologically related distractors were age invariant except in distractors that were both semantically and phonologically related. Only distractors semantically related to the picture interfered more for older than younger adults. These results are inconsistent with age-linked deficits in inhibition of irrelevant information from either internal or external sources. Rather, aging affects priming transmission in a connectionist network with asymmetric effects on semantic and phonological connections involved in comprehension and production, respectively. Research on language and aging has shown that some language functions are well maintained in old age, whereas others show pronounced age-related declines. Access to the meaning of words as reflected in vocabulary scores, for example, is relatively stable during adulthood

Do alternative names block young and older adults’ retrieval of proper names

by Emily S Cross , Deborah M Burke - Brain and Language , 2004
"... Abstract This study evaluates whether tip of the tongue experiences (TOTs) are caused by a more accessible word which blocks retrieval of the target word, especially for older adults. In a ''competitor priming'' paradigm, young and older adults produced the name of a famous char ..."
Abstract - Cited by 9 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract This study evaluates whether tip of the tongue experiences (TOTs) are caused by a more accessible word which blocks retrieval of the target word, especially for older adults. In a ''competitor priming'' paradigm, young and older adults produced the name of a famous character (e.g., Eliza Doolittle) in response to a question and subsequently named a picture of a famous actor or actress depicting this character (e.g., Audrey Hepburn as Eliza Doolittle). Older adults produced more TOTs than young adults, but prior production of a related character name did not affect TOTs, although it did reduce incorrect responses. There were no age differences in knowledge of films and TV and thus the age-related increase in TOTs is not because older adults have more relevant knowledge. The findings are compatible with models in which alternate words are a consequence not a cause of TOT.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...d that comes to mind first and suppresses the target word, preventing its retrieval (e.g., Anderson & Bjork, 1994; Brown, 1991; Jones, 1989; Reason & Lucas, 1984; Schacter, 1999). There is clear evidence that alternate words come repeatedly and involuntarily to mind and are experienced during many proper name TOTs that occur spontaneously in everyday life (Burke et al., 1991; Cohen & Faulkner, 1986). These recurring responses have been labeled ‘‘blockers,’’ but we will use the more theoretically neutral term ‘‘persistent alternates’’ because their functional effect on retrieval is in dispute (James & Burke, 2000; Meyer & Bock, 1992; Perfect & Hanley, 1992). Under the inhibition deficit (ID) hypothesis, persistent alternates would impair retrieval more for older than young adults because the ability to inhibit irrelevant information declines in old age. Irrelevant information, that is, persistent alternates in the case of TOTs, block retrieval of the target word until the alternate is inhibited (e.g., Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Zacks & Hasher, 1994). Within this framework, inhibition deficits at two E.S. Cross, D.M. Burke / Brain and Language 89 (2004) 174–181 175levels of processing are germane. Older adu...

Specific effects of aging on proper name retrieval: Now you see them, now you don’t

by Lori E. James - Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences , 2006
"... Existing research is equivocal as to whether aging negatively affects the retrieval of proper names more than other types of words. To clarify previous results, this study had young and older participants name and provide specific biographical information in response to pictures of familiar celebrit ..."
Abstract - Cited by 8 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
Existing research is equivocal as to whether aging negatively affects the retrieval of proper names more than other types of words. To clarify previous results, this study had young and older participants name and provide specific biographical information in response to pictures of familiar celebrities; it used two different measures of recall errors. An analysis of tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) responses indicated specific age-related increases in TOTs for names compared with words representing biographical information. An analysis including all other (non-TOT) recall errors did not indicate age-related deficits in name retrieval. These findings highlight the importance of the measures adopted in forming conclusions about age effects on the retrieval of names. F OR people of all ages, names are prone to retrieval failures,including tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states, which are those frustrating instances when we cannot produce well-known information even though we believe retrieval is imminent (e.g.,
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...n TOTs than did young adults, which would have eliminated the interaction. Indeed, many studies have shown age-related increases in TOTs for non-proper-name words (e.g., Heine, Ober, & Shenaut, 1999; =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-; White & Abrams, 2002). Furthermore, confidence intervals around the means for biographical-information TOTs do not include zero (see Figure 1). Nevertheless, the obtained interaction might result fr...

The effects of phonological neighborhoods on pronunciation variation in conversational speech

by Yao Yao , 2011
"... This dissertation investigates the effects of phonological neighborhoods on pronunciation variation in conversational speech. Phonological neighbors are defined as words that are different in one and only one phoneme by addition, deletion and substitution. Phonological neighborhood density refers to ..."
Abstract - Cited by 4 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
This dissertation investigates the effects of phonological neighborhoods on pronunciation variation in conversational speech. Phonological neighbors are defined as words that are different in one and only one phoneme by addition, deletion and substitution. Phonological neighborhood density refers to the number of neighbors a certain word has. Previous research has shown that phonological neighbors impede auditory perception, but facilitate lexical production. As a result, words from dense neighborhoods are harder to perceive, but easier to produce, than words from sparse neighborhoods. Following these effects, two opposite hypotheses can be formed regarding the effects of neighborhood density on pronunciation variation. The listener-oriented hypothesis predicts that high-density words will be hyperarticulated, in order to compensate for their perceptual difficulty. But the speaker-oriented hypothesis predicts that high-density words are more likely to undergo speech reduction, as other words that are easy to produce (such as high-frequency words). To test these hypotheses, two statistical models are constructed to investigate neighborhood effects on pronunciation variation in CVC monomorphemic content words. All speech data come from the Buckeye Corpus of Conversational Speech (Pitt, Dilley, Johnson, Kiesling,

Phonologically mediated priming of preexisting and new associations in young and older adults

by Katherine K. White, Lise Abrams - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 2004
"... Two experiments investigated phonologically mediated priming of preexisting and new associations in word retrieval. Young and older adults completed paired word stems with the first word that came to mind. Priming of preexisting associations occurred when word-stem pairs containing homophones (e.g., ..."
Abstract - Cited by 4 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
Two experiments investigated phonologically mediated priming of preexisting and new associations in word retrieval. Young and older adults completed paired word stems with the first word that came to mind. Priming of preexisting associations occurred when word-stem pairs containing homophones (e.g., beech-s____) showed more completions with the target (e.g., sand) relative to unrelated pairs (e.g., batch-s____), with more priming for subordinate than dominant homophones. Priming occurred for new associations independent of dominance, such that word-stem pairs containing homophones (e.g., beech-l____ _ and beach-l_____) were completed with the same word (e.g., laugh) more often than unrelated pairs (e.g., beech-l____ _ and batch-l_____). No age differences in phonologically mediated priming were found for either type of association, suggesting age equivalence in the utilization of bottom-up phonological connections. Research on phonologically mediated priming of visual stimuli has shown that a word’s meaning can be accessed through its phonology (Lesch & Pollatsek, 1993; Lukatela & Turvey, 1994). For example, people are faster to recognize the word sand when it is preceded by the word beech relative to an unrelated word such as batch, suggesting that the phonological code for beech is activated prior to its lexical representation.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...pictures. Furthermore, the finding that priming of older adults’sphonological representations was intact complements recentsresearch with phonological priming of tip-of-the-tongue (TOT)sstates (e.g., =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-; White & Abrams, 2002).sThese studies showed greater resolution of TOTs followingspresentation of the missing word’s phonology (see alsosAbrams, White, & Eitel, in press), and this increase insresolu...

New age-linked asymmetries: aging and the processing of familiar versus novel language on the input versus output side

by Lori E. James, Donald G. Mackay - Psychology and Aging , 2007
"... This research demonstrates 3 new age-linked asymmetries between identifying versus retrieving phono-logical information. Young and older adults read aloud familiar isolated words (e.g., mind) and novel pseudowords (e.g., mond) in a production task and identified lexical status for identical stimuli ..."
Abstract - Cited by 4 (2 self) - Add to MetaCart
This research demonstrates 3 new age-linked asymmetries between identifying versus retrieving phono-logical information. Young and older adults read aloud familiar isolated words (e.g., mind) and novel pseudowords (e.g., mond) in a production task and identified lexical status for identical stimuli in a comprehension task. Young adults made fewer errors than older adults in production but not compre-hension (an age-related input–output asymmetry), and they produced pseudowords but not words with fewer errors than older adults (a lexical-status asymmetry). The lexical-status asymmetry also occurred for response onset times but not for output durations (an onset–output asymmetry). All 3 asymmetries were predicted under the transmission deficit hypothesis (D. G. MacKay & D. M. Burke, 1990) but contradict theories such as general slowing that cannot explain why aging affects some types of information processing more than others.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

... words increases with aging in both diary studies (Burke, MacKay, Worthley, & Wade, 1991; Heine, Ober, & Shenault, 1999) and laboratory experiments (e.g., A. S. Brown & Nix, 1996; Burke et al., 1991; =-=James & Burke, 2000-=-; Maylor, 1990; White & Abrams, 2002), and older adults consider this “tip-of-the-tongue” (TOT) phenomenon one of their most troublesome problems (e.g., Cohen & Faulkner, 1986; Sunderland, Watts, Badd...

Memory in elderly people

by Jill D. Kester, Aaron S. Benjamin, Alan D. Castel, Fergus I. M. Craik - The Handbook of Memory Disorders (2 nd ed , 2002
"... Memory difficulty is one of the most common complaints of older adults (e.g. Hertzog & Dixon, 1994), so the study of age-related changes in memory function is motivated by practical as well as theoretical considerations. This chapter reviews the major empirical findings in memory and aging resea ..."
Abstract - Cited by 3 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
Memory difficulty is one of the most common complaints of older adults (e.g. Hertzog & Dixon, 1994), so the study of age-related changes in memory function is motivated by practical as well as theoretical considerations. This chapter reviews the major empirical findings in memory and aging research, with particular emphasis on recent work. In order to provide a contemporary context for understanding and evaluating the many results, we first summarize four major theoretical approaches to understanding memory and aging. At the end of the paper we will spell out the theories more fully, and discuss the extent to which each theory is able to accommodate current findings. The four theoretical approaches are a decline in processing speed, reduced processing resources, age-related inhibitory deficits, and decreased cognitive control. The slowing of behavior in old age is widely accepted and well documented (Birren, 1965; Salthouse, 1991, 1996). The basic notion is that decreased speed of mental processing underlies many if not all age-related cognitive deficits, either directly (i.e. behavior is slow and inefficient) or indirectly by disrupting the timing of a complex sequence of mental operations. Within this framework, then, age-related decrements in memory performance are not attributed to
Powered by: Apache Solr
  • About CiteSeerX
  • Submit and Index Documents
  • Privacy Policy
  • Help
  • Data
  • Source
  • Contact Us

Developed at and hosted by The College of Information Sciences and Technology

© 2007-2019 The Pennsylvania State University