• Documents
  • Authors
  • Tables
  • Log in
  • Sign up
  • MetaCart
  • DMCA
  • Donate

CiteSeerX logo

Advanced Search Include Citations
Advanced Search Include Citations

Temporal ‘‘bubbles’’ reveal key features for point-light biological motion perception. (2008)

by S M Thurman, E D Grossman
Venue:Journal of Vision,
Add To MetaCart

Tools

Sorted by:
Results 1 - 10 of 15
Next 10 →

Biological motion cues trigger reflexive attentional orienting.

by Jinfu Shi , Xuchu Weng , Sheng He , Yi Jiang - Cognition, , 2010
"... a b s t r a c t The human visual system is extremely sensitive to biological signals around us. In the current study, we demonstrate that biological motion walking direction can induce robust reflexive attentional orienting. Following a brief presentation of a central point-light walker walking tow ..."
Abstract - Cited by 6 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
a b s t r a c t The human visual system is extremely sensitive to biological signals around us. In the current study, we demonstrate that biological motion walking direction can induce robust reflexive attentional orienting. Following a brief presentation of a central point-light walker walking towards either the left or right direction, observers' performance was significantly better on a target in the walking direction compared with that in the opposite direction even when participants were explicitly told that walking direction was not predictive of target location. Interestingly, the effect disappeared when the walker was shown upside-down. Moreover, the reflexive attentional orienting could be extended to motions of other biological entities but not inanimate objects, and was not due to the viewpoint effect of the point-light figure. Our findings provide strong evidence that biological motion cues can trigger reflexive attentional orienting, and highlight the intrinsic sensitivity of the human visual attention system to biological signals.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...er & Murdoch, 1994; Troje, 2002), and identity information (Cutting & Kozlowski, 1977; Fani, Prasad, Harber, & Shiffrar,. All rights reserved.2005; Troje, Westhoff, & Lavrov, 2005) from point-light biological motion. Walking direction is another important attribute of biological motion, which provides critical information about another living creature’s disposition and intention. Previous studies have found that the walking direction can be successfully discriminated even when the point-light displays are embedded in dynamic visual noise (Bertenthal & Pinto, 1994; Neri, Morrone, & Burr, 1998; Thurman & Grossman, 2008). Walking direction information can be fully extracted in the peripheral vision (Thompson, Hansen, Hess, & Troje, 2007) and be processed incidentally (Thornton & Vuong, 2004). Developmental studies have also shown that young infants (6-month-old) are able to discriminate the walking direction of an upright point-light walker (Kuhlmeier, Troje, & Lee, 2010). Newly hatched chicks, lacking of any visual experience, tend to align their bodies in the apparent direction of the point-light animations (Vallortigara & Regolin, 2006). These findings suggest that there might be an intrinsic sensitivity t...

Review

by Bevra H Hahn, Jennifer Grossman, Benjamin J Ansell, Brian J Skaggs, Maureen Mcmahon , 2008
"... Altered lipoprotein metabolism in chronic inflammatory states: proinflammatory high-density lipoprotein and accelerated atherosclerosis in systemic lupus erythematosus and rheumatoid arthritis ..."
Abstract - Cited by 1 (0 self) - Add to MetaCart
Altered lipoprotein metabolism in chronic inflammatory states: proinflammatory high-density lipoprotein and accelerated atherosclerosis in systemic lupus erythematosus and rheumatoid arthritis
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...orizontal motion trajectories of the sequences, but left intact the vertical trajectories and horizontal mid-level features, which may be critical features in these sequences (Casile and Giese, 2005; =-=Thurman and Grossman, 2008-=-). It is likely that many of our ROIs, and in particular hMT+ and the STSp, contain populations of velocity-tuned neurons (e.g., Bruce et al., 1981), and our adaptation effect may have been due to tho...

ELEMENTS FOR A NEURAL THEORY OF THE PROCESSING OF DYNAMIC FACES

by Thomas Serre, Martin A. Giese, Thomas Serre, Martin A. Giese, Thomas Serre, Martin A. Giese
"... Face recognition has been a central topic in computer vision for at least two decades and progress in recent years has been significant. Automated face recognition systems are now widespread in applications ranging from surveillance to personal computers. In contrast, only a handful of neurobiologic ..."
Abstract - Cited by 1 (0 self) - Add to MetaCart
Face recognition has been a central topic in computer vision for at least two decades and progress in recent years has been significant. Automated face recognition systems are now widespread in applications ranging from surveillance to personal computers. In contrast, only a handful of neurobiologically plausible computational models have
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...es.sA central question in the context of such models has been how form and motionsprocessing contribute to the recognition of body motion. Consistent with experimentalsevidence (Casile & Giese, 2005; =-=Thurman & Grossman, 2008-=-; Vangeneugden, et al.,s2008), some models have proposed an integration of form and motion information,spotentially in the STS (Giese & Poggio, 2003; Peuskens, Vanrie, Verfaillie, & Orban,s2005).sConv...

Developmental tuning of reflexive attentional effect to biological motion cues

by Jing Zhao , Li Wang , Ying Wang , Xuchu Weng , Su Li , Yi Jiang
"... The human visual system is extremely sensitive to the direction information retrieved from biological motion. In the current study, we investigate the functional impact of this sensitivity on attentional orienting in young children. We found that children as early as 4 years old, like adults, showe ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
The human visual system is extremely sensitive to the direction information retrieved from biological motion. In the current study, we investigate the functional impact of this sensitivity on attentional orienting in young children. We found that children as early as 4 years old, like adults, showed a robust reflexive attentional orienting effect to the walking direction of an upright point-light walker, indicating that biological motion signals can automatically direct spatial attention at an early age. More importantly, the inversion effect associated with attentional orienting emerges by 4 years old and gradually develops into a similar pattern found in adults. These results provide strong evidence that biological motion cues can guide the distribution of spatial attention in young children, and highlight a critical development from a broadlyto finely-tuned process of utilizing biological motion cues in the human social brain. T he ability to quickly recognize the motion of biological entities in the environment is extremely important for a species' survival, as it enables one to hunt prey, avoid predation, assess the intention of an approaching rival, and even identify a potential mate. Numerous studies have demonstrated that human observers are remarkably adept at detecting and recognizing biological motion signals in complex visual scenes, even when they are portrayed by just a handful of point lights attached to the head and major joints 1 . Notably, the ability to attend preferentially to biological motion arises at a very early age. For example, infants as young as four months can differentiate between upright and inverted human point-light walkers 2 and show preference for the former 3 . Even newly hatched domestic chicks reared in the dark manifest a spontaneous preference for biological over nonbiological motion patterns 4 . A similar phenomenon has also been observed with human neonates. 2-or 3-day-old infants have been able to distinguish the point-light biological motion from other random rigid motion displays and prefer to look at the former even if it depicts the other species' shape (i.e., a walking hen) 20 . Intriguingly, peripheral walking direction information can automatically influence the processing of a centrally presented point-light walker

unknown title

by De Psicologia, Julho De, Bruno Armando, Aragão Henriques
"... ru ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
Abstract not found
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...on, 1988). However, when animated, the observers can easily perceive biological motion andsrecognize the stimuli hidden in the tangle of dots (Cutting et al., 1988; Thornton, Pinto, & Shiffrar,s1998; =-=Thurman & Grossman, 2008-=-). The most powerful example is the scrambled walker mask, inswhich the surrounding dots mimic local motion, which is to say, the mask contains not random motionsbut biological motion dots, such as mo...

Rufin Vogels

by J Vangeneugden , K Vancleef , T Jaeggli , L Vangool , R Vogels , 2010
"... A vast literature exists on human biological motion perception in impoverished displays, e.g., point-light walkers. Less is known about the perception of impoverished biological motion displays in macaques. We trained 3 macaques in the discrimination of facing direction (left versus right) and forw ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
A vast literature exists on human biological motion perception in impoverished displays, e.g., point-light walkers. Less is known about the perception of impoverished biological motion displays in macaques. We trained 3 macaques in the discrimination of facing direction (left versus right) and forward versus backward walking using motion-capture-based locomotion displays (treadmill walking) in which the body features were represented by cylinder-like primitives. The displays did not contain translatory motion. Discriminating forward versus backward locomotion requires motion information while the facing-direction/ view task can be solved using motion and/or form. All monkeys required lengthy training to learn the forward-backward task, while the view task was learned more quickly. Once acquired, the discriminations were specific to walking and stimulus format but generalized across actors. Although the view task could be solved using form cues, there was a small impact of motion. Performance in the forward-backward task was highly susceptible to degradations of spatiotemporal stimulus coherence and motion information. These results indicate that rhesus monkeys require extensive training in order to use the intrinsic motion cues related to forward versus backward locomotion and imply that extrapolation of observations concerning human perception of impoverished biological motion displays onto monkey perception needs to be made cautiously.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...ard from backward locomotions suggests that, unlike humans, monkeys do not spontaneously use such motion information to discriminate locomotion, but instead rely predominantly on spatial differences in trajectories and/or posture-related form cues. It is only after extensive training that the monkeys are able to use the motion information present in the displays. What sort of motion information did the monkeys in fact use to solve the forward–backward discrimination? The generalization results from the one-legged displays show that opponent motion, as postulated by Casile and Giese (2005) and Thurman and Grossman (2008), was not necessary, although it may have contributed to the discrimination performance of the “humanoid” displays by the monkeys (note that opponent motion might be important for the human perception of biological motion in more impoverished displays such as point-light stimuli, as suggested by Casile & Giese, 2005; Thurman & Grossman, 2008). As shown in Figure 1B, the direction of motion of the lower limbs differs between the forward and backward trajectories and such local, short-range motion direction signals (Braddick, 1974) might have been used by the animals to solve the task. Perturbat...

Composition du Jury:

by Aix Marseille Université, Dr. Constantin Frangoulis , 2012
"... Pour obtenir le grade de docteur en sciences de l’environnement. ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
Pour obtenir le grade de docteur en sciences de l’environnement.

Composition du jury

by École Doctorale Iaem Lorraine, Mauricio David, Cerda Villablanca, Bernard Girau (directeur Professeur, Université Henri Poincaré , 2011
"... UFR mathématiques et informatique ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
UFR mathématiques et informatique

Using eye tracking to investigate important cues for representative creature motion

by Meredith Mclendon, Ann Mcnamara, Tim Mclaughlin, Ravindra Dwivedi
"... Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee.

Adaptation des représentations internes de l’action à la microgravité: continuum fonctionnel de la perception à l’exécution

by Pierre-Yives Chabeauti , 2012
"... ..."
Abstract - Add to MetaCart
Abstract not found
Powered by: Apache Solr
  • About CiteSeerX
  • Submit and Index Documents
  • Privacy Policy
  • Help
  • Data
  • Source
  • Contact Us

Developed at and hosted by The College of Information Sciences and Technology

© 2007-2019 The Pennsylvania State University